Deliver Your News to the World

Capuchin Monkeys Caught on Camera With Abducted Baby Howler Monkeys

What Is the Reason for This Behavior?


WEBWIRE

On Jicarón Island, in the Coiba National Park, off the western Pacific coast of Panama, white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus imitator) are the subject of a long-term observational study that began in 2017, when researchers from the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute (STRI) and Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior (MPI-AB) noticed a group of them habitually using stones as tools. To document this, they installed motion-triggered cameras, or camera traps, around the island to observe this and other behaviors.

Another primate species on the island are howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata coibensis); their name refers to their loud howls that can be heard up to 3 miles away (around 4.82 kilometers). Capuchins and howlers cohabitate mostly without issue because their diets are different, and therefore they do not compete for food.

However, since 2022, some of the immature male capuchin monkeys from the tool-using group have been caught on camera carrying infant howler monkeys.

Doctoral researcher Zoë Goldsborough of MPI-AB noticed this around June 2022 while looking through the camera traps’ footage. The first sighting of this behavior was captured as early as January of that year. She alerted her advisor and group leader, Brendan Barrett, and dug through the tens of thousands of images and videos collected by the cameras to find if there was more evidence of this occurring on the island.

“We had all the footage from cameras on Jicarón recording the tool-using monkeys for the whole year, so we could reconstruct the scene to see if this weird behavior was just a one-off, or something bigger,” Barrett said.

Goldsborough found evidence of four different howler infants being carried, nearly always by the same individual, a subadult male from the tool-using group that the researchers named Joker. But how did Joker get these infants, and why did he carry them for days at a time?

“At first, we thought it could be adoption,” Goldsborough said, referring to the anecdotes of some animals adopting babies of other species. But in most of those cases, adoption and caring of abandoned infants is carried out by females. In this case, the carrier was male. “The fact that a male was the exclusive carrier of these babies was an important piece of the puzzle.”

For months afterward, there was no evidence of capuchins carrying howlers. “We’d decided that it was one individual trying something new, which is not uncommon to see among capuchins,” Barrett said. “These are deeply curious animals who are constantly exploring the forest and figuring out how they can interact with their world.”

Five months later, however, the researchers discovered images and videos of more howler infants being carried. This time it was not just Joker; other young male capuchins were carrying baby howlers as well. 

They continued to analyze the footage, and found that in the span of 15 months, 11 infant howler monkeys were carried by juvenile or subadult male capuchin monkeys for periods of up to nine days. Howler babies clung to the backs or bellies of their carriers, who appeared to be going about their normal business of travelling or using tools to crack open nuts and other food sources. The researchers gathered the footage on an interactive website documenting how this random behavior spread from one individual to a group of other young males at an increasing speed.

“This has never been observed anywhere else, not on this island, or in any other populations of capuchin monkeys,” Goldsborough said. “We also did not find any evidence of something like this occurring between other species.”

As is often the case in the natural world, one animal’s loss is another animal’s gain. But in this case, abducting and carrying infants from a different species seems to provide no gain or benefit for the capuchin monkeys.

So why do they do this? And why do they not abduct infants from their own species?

“This is something we don’t have a clear answer for,” Goldsborough said. “A possible explanation can lie in the more docile nature of howler monkeys compared to capuchins, which has been reported in other sites where capuchins often harass howlers. I imagine that stealing an infant from a capuchin would be riskier than from a howler. But this would also be an entirely different behavior since the group dynamics then also play a role, and capuchin groups are also much bigger than howler groups.”

The authors describe this behavior as a social tradition or cultural fad, spreading in a population through social learning and in parallel to trends observed in other animals, such as chimpanzees wearing a blade of grass in their ears like an accessory.

The implications, however, are deeper. The howler babies, all less than 4 weeks old, appear to have been abducted from their parents. Howler parents were seen or heard on camera calling for their missing infants from nearby trees.

At that age, the infant howlers do not have much of a chance of survival; four of the babies were observed to have died, presumably of malnourishment, and the researchers suspect that none of them survived.

“The capuchins didn’t hurt the babies, but they couldn’t provide the milk that infants need to survive,” Goldsborough said. “What we see from the howler infants who did die, is that they were all carried until they became too weak to cling, then were often carried in the capuchin’s hand for a bit more, and some even after they died for a day or so. We can only assume that the same happened to the other infants, and that they were eventually left somewhere once they were dead or close to dying.”

Interestingly, this group of howler-kidnapping capuchins, which is also the group using tools on Jicarón, are only males, hinting that these two socially learned traditions might spring from the same source: boredom.

“Survival appears easy on Jicarón,” said Meg Crofoot, managing director of MPI-AB, one of the founders of this project and a former fellow at STRI. “There are no predators and few competitors, which gives capuchins lots of time and little to do. It seems this ‘luxurious’ life set the scene for these social animals to be innovators. This new tradition shows us that necessity need not be the mother of invention. For a highly intelligent monkey living in a safe, perhaps even under-stimulating environment, boredom and free time might be sufficient.”

This study was led by a team from the MPI-AB in Germany, STRI in Panama, Universidad del Rosario in Colombia and Ithaca College in the United States. The study’s camera-trapping period ran from January 2022 to July 2023, and the team does not know to what extent the tradition persisted afterward since all the data has not yet been analyzed. But if this behavior spreads to other capuchin groups or continues to impact howlers, which are an endangered species on Jicarón, it could become a conservation issue in Coiba National Park.

“Witnessing the spread of this behavior had a profound effect on all of us,” Crofoot said. “We feel even more responsible to keep learning from this natural population of primates who, to our knowledge, are the only ones on Earth to be practicing this strange tradition.”


( Press Release Image: https://photos.webwire.com/prmedia/7/338587/338587-1.jpg )


WebWireID338587





This news content was configured by WebWire editorial staff. Linking is permitted.

News Release Distribution and Press Release Distribution Services Provided by WebWire.